Unit 5 Overview.doc_第1页
Unit 5 Overview.doc_第2页
Unit 5 Overview.doc_第3页
Unit 5 Overview.doc_第4页
Unit 5 Overview.doc_第5页
已阅读5页,还剩16页未读 继续免费阅读

下载本文档

版权说明:本文档由用户提供并上传,收益归属内容提供方,若内容存在侵权,请进行举报或认领

文档简介

Unit 5 Overview Read Toy Story by David Cohen Take a test on fast reading Read Watch and Learn by Gregg Easterbrook Read a passage about shopping with children Toy Storyby David Cohen Advertisers like to take advantage of childrens natural credulity. In a world full of dazzling advertisements, how do children react to them? Have they learned how to weigh up advertising before they succumb to its promises? “Whys that man got his hand up a sock? Dont they know how to do it properly? Thats not going to get me to buy it, is it?” carped one seven-year-old about the glove puppets used in Burger Kings The Lost World television advertisement. Call it cynicism. Call it sophistication. Nowadays its not unusual to find children as young as four making judgments like these, claims Nicky Buss of the advertising agency Ammirati Puris Lintas in London. By then children are “brand literate” and they can see through “marketing hyperbole”. Or can they? Is advertising geared at children even ethical? Since the 1970s, the battle between the forces for and against child advertising has hinged on whether or not kids understand the motive behind advertising. The debate is far from over, but new studies suggest that Buss is onto something. Either children are getting wise to the advertising game, and at an earlier age than in the 1970s and 1980s, or in the past psychologists underestimated their young subjects ability to work out other peoples motivation. It doesnt take a genius to work out why advertisers are trying harder to market directly to children. British children aged four to fourteen spend an average of ?2.49 each week. This makes the pocket money market worth more than?1.5 billion a year, according to a recent report from management consultancy Datamonitor. In the US the pocket money market is worth a massive $64 billion a year. Selling to children has become big business, and advertisers want to make it as scientific as possible. Winthrop Publications in London has just launched the International Journal of Advertising and Marketing to Children. One article reports that 60 per cent of children aged two to eleven know by the end of October what they want for Christmas, and that for girls under seven the biggest deciding factor is what they see on television. Conferences and consultancies abound. Pay 2000 and you can attend Kid Power 99 at any one of a string of European venues. The meetings offer workshops on “what works with kids and why”, “peer group marketing” and how to “think like a kid”. Consultancy firms will tell you how to build “a wall of communication” to influence “your core consumer lifestyle” from the moment said consumer is two years old. Meanwhile the London-based Childrens Research Unit (CRU), a for-profit organisation, surveys the tastes of 7 000 children, three times a year in their schools, via the Internet. Children are notoriously fickle, and advertisers have a hard job keeping up with their capricious tastes, explains Glen Smith, the units director and a psychologist who also edits the International Journal of Advertising and Marketing to Children. For a fee, market researchers can buy “hot spots” in the survey, he says. There are, of course, regulations in most countries specifically to protect the child consumer. In the UK, “advertising must not take advantage of childrens natural credulity and loyalty and must not arouse unreasonable expectations of toys and games by special effects”, says Helena Hunt of the Independent Television Commission (ITC). “Children must also not feel under pressure to buy.” And the ITC Code works, according to the Advertising Associations James Aitchison. “Less than 1 per cent of complaints received by the Advertising Association in 1998 related to ads for toys or games,” he says. But what counts as taking advantage of a childs natural credulity? And isnt that an impossible standard to meet, if a child does not even grasp the notion that ads are trying to sell something? If on the other hand, children are aware of the purpose of ads, those aimed at children are no more sinister than those aimed at adults. The backdrop to todays research on kids and advertising is the cognitive theory put forward over 70 years ago by the famous Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget. According to Piaget, children go through four stages of cognitive development a model that, with relatively modest tweaking, still dominates child psychology today. Between two and seven years old, children are in the “pre-operational” stage. Completely egocentric, they are at the mercy of their immediate perceptions. They lack what psychologists call a “theory of mind”: they dont understand that the world looks different from another persons perspective, or that other people can have motives and desires that differ or even clash with their own. They certainly couldnt be expected to realise that an advertisement was a manifestation of those different motives. After seven, children enter the “concrete operational” stage: they become less egocentric, are capable of more structured thinking, and understand that the world is not always as it seems to their immediate perceptions. Had Piaget ever considered children and advertising (and its likely he would have thought it beneath him, being more concerned with such heady questions as how children solve syllogisms), he would have argued that they had no clue as to the motives behind the media until well into the concrete operational stage. The first studies on childrens understanding of advertising seemed to fit Piagets model. One 25-year-old study found that 96 per cent of five to six year olds, 85 per cent of eight to nine year olds and 62 per cent of eleven to twelve year olds “do not fully understand the purpose of TV advertising”. At the time, psychologists in the US used those findings in their bid to press the US Federal Trade Commission to ban toy ads on TV, on the basis that children under the age of eight didnt understand “the commercial meaning” of ads. Advertisers persuaded the FTC against a ban. But in the 1990s similar arguments led to a ban on toy ads on TV in Greece and Sweden. Swedish law bans any product ads that aim to attract the attention of the under-twelves. Now the European advertising industry wants to see those bans lifted, or at the very least to ensure that they dont spread. Ironically, some psychologists who might usually be more comfortable arguing against sales pitches to children have had to concede that are shrewder than they once supposed. Take Jeffrey Goldstein, a psychologist at Utrecht University in the Netherlands. Goldstein, who also writes reports on the latest research into children and television for companies such as Nintendo and Compaq, believes that the test used in the 25-year-old study was too stringent. To be deemed “fully aware”, kids had to explain verbally that ads were trying to sell something and make money out of children. Psychologist Henry Wellman, of the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor, argues that Piagets framework does not fit the modern child. “Children nowadays are exposed to a much wider range of social interaction. They go to day care; they dont live in nuclear families. They engage much more in pretend play,” Wellman says. Children communicate with far more people at earlier ages than they did in the past. It grows them up. According to Wellmans analysis of thousands of childrens conversations, “around the age of three to four children understand that what you display on your face doesnt necessarily go with your internal state. By the age of five to six, most children understand you can deceive people by showing one kind of overt behaviour and feeling something else.” If you understand deception, you are well on the way to understanding advertising. Piaget may have chosen just the experimental subjects to exacerbate any difference in the cognitive abilities of children of the 1920s and of the present day. He based most of his key ideas on observations of his own children Jacqueline, Laurent and Lucienne whose upbringing was very sheltered. His studies also suffered from a problem that plagues child psychology to this day: how do you get a child to make explicit what may be an implicit understanding? One way to tease you out what children implicitly understand about advertising is to see whether they realise that TV ads are different from regular programmes. In one such experiment, 66 children aged between four and eight years old watched two sorts of ads. The genuine version extolled a face cream on the basis that it made users better looking. A doctored version praised the cream but the punch line was that it gave users disgusting spots. The children were asked which they preferred and why. Children aged four to five liked the funny endings better and did not notice whether or not the punch lines made commercial sense. All the eight-year-olds were totally familiar with the advertising game. They laughed at the doctored ads not just because they were funny but because they were pathetic as ads. A face cream that gives you spots is not a product that will sell, they pointed out. But it was the reactions of six-year-olds that revealed most. Just over half understood that there was something wrong with the funny endings, even though they couldnt always say just what. That suggests that many six-year-olds only have a limited understating of what ads are trying to achieve, says psychologist Brian Young of the University of Exeter, who reported the findings at the 1998 British Psychological Society conference. An experiment conducted by the CRU reaches a different conclusion. Four-year-old children were shown TV commercials and were asked using dolls representing mum, dad, children and so on to select the doll that the advert was talking to. “We found if there was a frozen pea commercial they moved the mum forward, if it was a toy they moved the child doll forward,” Smith says. If four-year-olds understand whom a sales pitch is aimed at, he says, it is reasonable to assume they have some implicit understanding of advertisings goal. Smith has not published this research because, like much of the CRUs output, it is only available to clients. Smith says his study and others reassure him that advertising to children is not “sinful or wicked”, but, he concedes, one should “be mindful of the gullibility of young children”. Although the growing consensus is that by the age of five many children realise there is something different about ads, some psychologists claim they still do not truly understand the purpose of advertising. How can they, asks Young, when they dont even know how to “sell” them selves? “A number of studies,” he says, “show its only around the age of seven that children get a sense of promoting themselves. For example, if you ask six-year-olds to put themselves forward to become one of a team they tell you about themselves warts and all.” Only around seven still young, according to Piagets theories do children understand that if they want to convince others to have them on their team, they need to accentuate the positive and eliminate the negative in the best tradition of Madison Avenue. It might seem as if recent studies on childrens grasp of advertising are fuelling the debate rather than settling it. But one clear trend has emerged and one thats troubling for advertisers. Almost as soon as children understand what advertising is about they become hostile to it. One study of girls and boys aged seven to eleven in Aberystwyth, Wales, found the children to be not only knowing but dismissive of TV ads. Even seven-year-olds “responded with surprising hostility”, says Merris Griffiths, a child psychologist at the University of Wales in Aberystwyth. “They felt insulted by the ads. They say things like This is trickery. The girls had the most hostile reactions. Youd think none of them had played with a Barbie doll.” Then there was that seven-year-olds put-a-sock-in-it comment. Advertisers and marketers, it seems, have every reason to get a severe case of jitters whenever they plan a new Beanie Babies or Pokmon sales campaign. After all, says Goldstein, “theres a conference every day on marketing to kids. If advertisers really knew how to sell to children, they wouldnt be doing that.” (1984 words)D. Discussing the following topics. 1. Why do advertisers try hard to market directly to children?答案:It doesnt take a genius to work out why advertisers try hard to market directly to children. British children aged four to fourteen spend an average of 2.49 each week. This makes the pocket money market worth more than 1.5 billion a year, according to a recent report from management consultancy Datamonitor. In the US the pocket money market is worth a massive $64 billion a year. Selling to children has become big business.2. How is child advertising made as scientific as possible? 答案:Many measures have been taken to achieve this purpose. For example, Winthrop Publications in London has just launched the International Journal of Advertising and Marketing to Children. One article reports that 60 per cent of children aged two to eleven know by the end of October what they want for Christmas, and that for girls under seven the biggest deciding factor is what they see on television. Conferences and consultancies abound. Pay2000 and you can attend Kid Power 99 at any one of a string of European venues. The meetings offer workshops on what works with kids and why, peer group1 marketing and how to think like a kid. Consultancy firms will tell you how to build a wall of communication to influence your core consumer lifestyle from the moment said consumer is two years old.3. What can we learn from psychologists experiments on childrens response to ads? 答案: From psychologists experiments about childrens response to ads, we can learn that children are not as gullible as they are thought to be. They become more cynical and sophisticated. Children are brand literate and they can see through marketing hyperbole. Either children are getting wise to the advertising game, and at an earlier age than in the 1970s and 1980s, or in the past psychologists underestimated their young subjects ability to work out other peoples motivation. From article reports we know that 60 per cent of children aged two to eleven know by the end of October know what they want for Christmas, and that for girls under seven the biggest deciding factor is what they see on television. Children are notoriously fickle, and advertisers have a hard job keeping up with their capricious tastes. Children are aware of the purpose of ads, those aimed children are no more sinister than those aimed at adults.Text 2 Watch and Learn by Gregg Easterbrook In the days after the Colorado slaughter, discussion of violent images in American culture was dominated by the canned positions of the anti-Hollywood right and the mammon-is-our-God film lobby. The debate missed three vital points: the distinction between what adults should be allowed to see (anything) and what the inchoate minds of children and adolescents should see; the way in which important liberal battles to win free expression in art and literature have been perverted into an excuse for antisocial video brutality produced by cynical capitalists; and the difference between censorship and voluntary acts of responsibility. The day after the Colorado shooting, Mike De Luca, an executive of New Line Cinema, told USA Today that when kids kill, “bad home life, bad parenting, having guns in the home” are “more of a factor than what we put out there for entertainment.” Setting aside the disclosure that Hollywood now categorizes scenes of movie stars gunning down the innocent as “entertainment,” De Luca is correct; studies do show that upbringing is more determinant of violent behavior than any other factor. But research also clearly shows that the viewing of violence can cause aggression and crime. So the question is: In a society already plagued by poor parenting and only slightly limited gun sales, why does the entertainment industry feel privileged to make violence even more prevalent? Even when researchers factor out other influences such as parental attention, many peer-reviewed studies have found casual links between viewing phony violence and engaging in actual violence. A 1971 surgeon generals report asserted a broad relationship between the two. Studies by Brandon Centerwall, an epidemiologist at the University of Wisconsin, have shown that the post war murder rise in the United States began roughly a decade after TV viewing became common. Centerwall also found that in South Africa, where television was not generally available until 1975, national murder rates started rising about a decade later. Leonard Eron, a psychologist at the University of Michigan, has been tracking video violence and actual violence for almost four decades. His initial studies in 1960 found that even the occa sional violence depicted in 1950s television to which every parent would gladly return today caused increased aggression among eight-year-olds. By the adult years, Erons studies find, those who watched the most TV and movies in childhood were much more likely to have been arrested for, or convicted of, violent felonies. Eron believes that 10 percent of U.S. violent crime is caused by exposure to images of violence, meaning that 90 percent is not, but that a 10 percent national reduction in violence might be achieved merely by moderating the content of television and movies. “Kids learn by observation,” Eron says. “If what they o

温馨提示

  • 1. 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。图纸软件为CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.压缩文件请下载最新的WinRAR软件解压。
  • 2. 本站的文档不包含任何第三方提供的附件图纸等,如果需要附件,请联系上传者。文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
  • 3. 本站RAR压缩包中若带图纸,网页内容里面会有图纸预览,若没有图纸预览就没有图纸。
  • 4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
  • 5. 人人文库网仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对用户上传分享的文档内容本身不做任何修改或编辑,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
  • 6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
  • 7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。

评论

0/150

提交评论