




已阅读5页,还剩6页未读, 继续免费阅读
版权说明:本文档由用户提供并上传,收益归属内容提供方,若内容存在侵权,请进行举报或认领
文档简介
JCPS-00396; No. of pages: 10; 4C:Available online at ScienceDirectJournal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx xxxResearch ArticleSensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A reviewand introductionAradhna Krishna a, Norbert Schwarz b , c ,aRoss School of Business, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USAbDepartment of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USAcMarshall School of Business, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USAReceived 22 December 2013; accepted 23 December 2013AbstractThere has been a recent swell of interest in marketing as well as psychology pertaining to the role of sensory experiences in judgment anddecision making. Within marketing, the eld of sensory marketing has developed which explores the role of the senses in consumer behavior. Inpsychology, the dominant computer metaphor of information processing has been challenged by researchers demonstrating various manners inwhich mental activity is grounded in sensory experience. These ndings are arduous to explain using the amodal model of the human mind. In thisintroduction, we rst delineate key assumptions of the information processing paradigm and then discuss some of the key conceptual challengesposed by the research generally appearing under the titles of embodiment, grounded cognition, or sensory marketing. We then address the use ofbodily feelings as a source of information; next, we turn to the role of context sensitive perception, imagery, and simulation in consumer behavior,and nally discuss the role of metaphors. Through this discourse, we note the contributions to the present special issue as applicable. 2013 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.IntroductionIn recent years, the role of sensory experiences in judgmentand decision making has seen a surge of interest in marketing aswell as psychology. In marketing, scattered research on the roleof the senses in consumer behavior has been brought togetherunder the rubric of sensory marketing, that is, “marketing thatengages the consumers senses and affects their perception,judgment, and behavior” (Krishna, 2012, p. 332; for reviews, seeKrishna, 2012, 2013, and the contributions in Krishna, 2010). Inpsychology, researchers challenged the dominant computermetaphor of information processing by highlighting ways inwhich mental activity is grounded in sensory experience(for reviews, see Barsalou, 2008; Meier, Schnall, Schwarz, &Bargh, 2012; Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber,& Ric, 2005). The rapidly accumulating empirical findings are Corresponding author at: Dept of Psychology, University of SouthernCalifornia, 3620 S. McClintock Ave, Los Angeles, CA 900891061, USA.E-mail address: (N. Schwarz).often surprising and difficult to account for within the amodalmodel of the human mind that has dominated psychology andconsumer research since the cognitive revolution of the 1970s.The findings also amplify earlier challenges to models of rationalchoice anybody who was dismayed by the observation thatdifferent ways of presenting a choice alternative can affect choice(for reviews, see the contributions in Lichtenstein & Slovic, 2006)will be even more dismayed by the extent to which incidentalexposure to sensory experiences in one domain can affect choicein another, substantively unrelated domain (for a review, see Lee& Schwarz, 2014).As the history of science illustrates, at early stages of aresearch program the focus is often on identifying challenges todominant paradigms in the hope that they will capture a fieldsattention (Root-Bernstein, 1989). At that stage, documentingnovel phenomena that are difficult to account for by the dominantparadigm takes precedence over understanding the details of howthe new phenomena operate. Consistent with this regularity, early“embodiment” experiments focused on highlighting findings thatone would not expect on the basis of traditional models of1057-7408/$ -see front matter 2013 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved./10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.006Please cite this article as: Krishna, A., & Schwarz, N., Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A review and introduction, Journal of ConsumerPsychology (2014), /10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.006Electronic copy available at: /abstract=25499632A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxxxxxjudgment and decision making, whereas distinctions betweendifferent theoretical approaches to embodiment received lessattention (for an early discussion, see Anderson, 2008). Morerecently, researchers began testing competing theories of theunderlying processes; history suggests that this will be the focusof future research in this area. At present, however, there is wideconsensus that we cannot understand human cognition withouttaking into account that humans interact with the world throughtheir senses and do their thinking within a body; but there is muchless consensus about what this implies and how it is bestconceptualized. Moreover, the different emerging theoreticalperspectives are not necessarily mutually exclusive and differentprocesses may contribute to observed effects under differentconditions.Our introduction to this special issue of the Journal ofConsumer Psychology summarizes these developments, notesdifferent theoretical perspectives, and places the contributionsto the special issue in their context. We begin by revisiting keyassumptions of the information processing paradigm thathas dominated consumer psychology for the last four decadesand note some of the conceptual challenges posed by recentresearch commonly presented under the headings of embodi-ment, grounded cognition, or sensory marketing. As thisresearch moves beyond initial attention-grabbing illustrations,researchers are increasingly identifying limiting conditionsand competing pathways, a step that is central to understand-ing the phenomena and the conditions under which they can(or cannot) be reliably replicated. While the often counterin-tuitive and amusing nature of some of the iconic findings addsto their popular interest value, it also distracts casual observers(and reviewers) from the fact that the findings pose seriouschallenges to how we think about the human mind. Next, wenote these conceptual challenges.Information processing, with and without a body:a historical perspectiveIn the 1970s, the development of general models ofinformation processing provided an integrative framework forthe conceptualization of human memory, judgment, anddecision making (for authoritative contemporary treatments,see Lachman, Lachman, & Butterfield, 1979; Wyer, 1974). In anutshell, the approach assumed that people acquire informationthrough their senses and that this information is translated into acommon code that is independent of the modality in which theinformation was initially acquired. These amodal representa-tions were assumed to be the building blocks of knowledge(semantic memory), on which the mind operates with generalprocedures that are also independent of the initial modality.Once stored, knowledge representations were assumed to becontext independent, although context could influence howeasily a stored representation could be accessed. Theseassumptions were consistent with a computer metaphor thatemphasized encoding, storage, and retrieval operations andtook it for granted that all operations require a common format,paralleling the operational requirements of a computer. Theiremphasis on amodal knowledge representation was a sharpbreak with centuries of earlier theorizing that took it for grantedthat visual, olfactory or haptic inputs were stored in a formthat reflected the modality of their acquisition. In fact, havinga controversial discussion about whether imagery involvesanything like an image (e.g., Paivio, 1971; Pylyshyn, 1973)would have been unimagineable prior to the informationprocessing paradigm (and the controversy has been settled infavor of modal representations; see Kosslyn, Thompson, &Ganis, 2006).While the information processing paradigm facilitatedenormous progress in psychological research, it also camewith its own blind spots (for a discussion, see Schwarz, 2000).Most notably, it neglected phenomena that were not easilyconceptualized within a computer metaphor, including the roleof moods, emotions, and other subjective experiences as well asthe role of motivation, which was reduced to processing goals.Given their subject matter, these limitations were particularlyapparent to social psychologists, who integrated the role offeelings and “hot” motivational processes in the social cognitionvariants of information processing models, paying tribute tosocial psychologys long tradition of attending to visceral aspectsof social perception and behavior (for reviews, see Schwarz &Clore, 2007; Strack & Deutsch, 2007). For the most part,however, social cognition theories did not fundamentallychallenge the assumption of amodal representations. Instead,feelings and bodily states were themselves assumed to be storedin memory in an amodal form, for example, as nodes in asemantic network (e.g., Bower, 1981), although they provided“online” information that competed with the amodally represent-ed “offline” information stored in memory (e.g., Schwarz, 1990).Some current embodiment research is compatible with thistradition and addresses how concurrent bodily experiences serveas a source of information in judgment and choice; theseprocesses can be conceptualized within feelings-as-informationtheory (Schwarz, 2012) and we address this work in the sectionon bodily feelings as information.Going beyond these extensions of the information process-ing paradigm, the assumption of a largely decontextualized andamodal mind has been questioned more radically by severalstreams of research that converge in support of an emergingmetatheoretical narrative that emphasizes the situated, experien-tial, and embodied nature of human cognition. Following earlyinsights of William James (1890), situated cognition researchassumes that thinking is for doing and that any mind worthhaving needs to be geared towards facilitating action in ways thatare sensitive to the requirement of ones current situation (for areview, see Smith & Semin, 2004). This perspective turnscontextual influences on judgment and choice from deplorable“context dependency” into laudable “context sensitivity”, with farreaching consequences for the conceptualization of preferences,attitudes, and related concepts (for a discussion, see Schwarz,2007). Because people experience the world through their senses,sensory information and the accompanying subjective experi-ences play a key role in human action and cognition, even indomains that seem abstract and far removed from immediatesensory inputs. Some suggest that this reflects that humans“evolved from creatures whose neural resources were devotedPlease cite this article as: Krishna, A., & Schwarz, N., Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A review and introduction, Journal of ConsumerPsychology (2014), /10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.006Electronic copy available at: /abstract=2549963A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxxxxx3primarily to perceptual and motoric processing” (Wilson, 2002,p. 625). The later development of higher mental processespresumably took advantage of these evolutionarily olderprograms and reused them for new purposes (Anderson, 2010).From this perspective, what psychologists traditionally referredto as “higher mental processes” is grounded in bodily experiencebecause it reuses phylogenetically older neural circuitry (for areview of supporting evidence, see Anderson, 2010). Othersemphasize ontogenetic rather than phylogenetic learning andsuggest that early sensory experience with the physical and socialworld (e.g., feeling warm and safe in the presence of a caregiver)structures later thinking about abstract concepts (e.g., security),resulting in associations between sensory experience and abstractthought across content domains (e.g., Williams, Huang, & Bargh,2009).Independent of specific assumptions about their origin, linksbetween sensory experience and abstract concepts are oftenreflected in widely shared metaphors (for a review, see Landau,Meier, & Keefer, 2010). For example, a relationship may bedescribed as “cold” or “warm” and, indeed, experiencingphysical warmth through touching a warm object increases theperception of social warmth in others (e.g., Williams & Bargh,2008), much as the social experience of a “cold shoulder”influences perceptions of the physical temperature of the room(e.g., Zhong & Leonardelli, 2008). A large part of currentembodiment research in consumer psychology addresses suchmetaphorical influences; we discuss key findings and concep-tual controversies in the section on metaphors.Finally, the most radical approach to grounded cognitionconceptualizes all mental acts as acts of modality specific sensorysimulation (e.g., Barsalou, 1999; Barsalou, Niedenthal, Barbey,& Ruppert, 2003). From this perspective, “as an experienceoccurs (e.g., easing into a chair), the brain captures states acrossthe modalities and integrates them with a multimodal represen-tation stored in memory (e.g., how a chair looks and feels, theaction of sitting, introspections of comfort and relaxation)”(Barsalou, 2008, p. 618). Such multimodal representationscan be activated through any of the participating modalities.More important, thinking about a chair is assumed to involve asimulation of previous sensory experience, making simula-tions of earlier experience an essential part of any mentalactivity. Supporting this rationale, a large body of neuroim-aging findings shows that supposedly amodal “high level”processes such as language comprehension, categoriza-tion, or retrieval entail activation of the brain areasinvolved in “low level” sensory processing (for a review, seeBarsalou, 2008). This approach is increasingly informingresearch in sensory marketing that explores the role of mentalsimulation in advertising and related issues (e.g., Elder &Krishna, 2012), as reviewed below.The next three sections elaborate on these core themes.We first address the use of bodily feelings as a source ofinformation, which is most compatible with traditional socialcognition models; next, we turn to the role of context sensitiveperception, imagery, and simulation in consumer behavior, andfinally discuss the role of metaphors. Throughout, we note thecontributions to the present special issue as applicable.Bodily experience as a source of informationThe assumption that people attend to their own bodily statesand extract information from them has a long tradition in art,philosophy, and psychology. From the heart that beats fasterwhen seeing the loved one to the cold sweat of fear, perceptionsof bodily states figure prominently in poems, songs, andpopular images. Experimental studies confirm that bodily statesinform human judgment and that people use this information asthey use any other information (for reviews, see Herbert &Pollatos, 2012; Schwarz & Clore, 2007). Consistent with thelogic of feelings-as-information theory (for a recent review, seeSchwarz, 2012), the impact of bodily states depends on theirperceived informational value; it is attenuated or eliminatedwhen the experience is attributed to an unrelated source, andenhanced when it is experienced despite contrary influences. Afast beating heart indeed increases the attractiveness of pin-upphotos, unless one realizes that the elevated heart beat may bethe result of having climbed several flights of stairs (Zillman,1978). Similarly, physiological symptoms of fear increase theimpact of a fear-arousing movie, in particular when they areexperienced despite having taken an allegedly tranquilizingpill; yet their influence is eliminated when the symptoms can beattributed to an allegedly arousing pill (Schwarz, Servay, &Kumpf, 1985).Facilitating or impairing a persons bodily response to astimulus also facilitates or impairs the persons subjectiveexperience of the stimulus. As Strack, Martin, and Stepper(1988) showed by varying the way in which participants held apen in their mouths, a cartoon is perceived as funnier when theviewers face can respond with a smile than when pursed lipsimpair a smile, highlighting that facial muscle feedback plays acrucial role in subjective experience and judgment. In recentyears, many consumers voluntarily exposed themselves to atreatment that impairs facial muscle response, namely botoxinjections. The unanticipated side-effects range from impairedaffective experience (Davis, Senghas, Brandt, & Ochsner, 2010)to an impaired understanding of others emotional expressions(Neil & Chartrand, 2011) and impaired processing of emotionwords (Havas, Glenberg, Gutowski, Lucarelli, & Davidson,2010). The latter findings show that understanding othersemotional expression, or merely comprehending emotionallycharged words, involves activity of
温馨提示
- 1. 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。图纸软件为CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.压缩文件请下载最新的WinRAR软件解压。
- 2. 本站的文档不包含任何第三方提供的附件图纸等,如果需要附件,请联系上传者。文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
- 3. 本站RAR压缩包中若带图纸,网页内容里面会有图纸预览,若没有图纸预览就没有图纸。
- 4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
- 5. 人人文库网仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对用户上传分享的文档内容本身不做任何修改或编辑,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
- 6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
- 7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。
最新文档
- 提成合作协议书合同范本
- 2025版跨境电商物流服务合同标准范本
- 2025版外贸建筑材料买卖合同
- 2025年度高科技园区合作普通合伙合同
- 2025年度智能停车场建设与运营管理合同
- 2025版石材运输绿色环保合同范本
- 2025版化工原料运输合同标的防腐蚀与安全
- 2025年度私人地下室租赁合同附赠水电安装服务协议
- 2025版精细化管理砌筑班组劳务分包合同
- 2025版汕头住宅租赁合同范本涵盖租赁房屋维修费用分摊
- 神经根型腰椎病课件
- 食品行业质量控制管理制度
- (完整版)康复诊疗指南及规范
- 五年级下册黑布林英语阅读10篇
- 2025届四川省宜宾市叙州区二中生物高一第一学期期末学业水平测试试题含解析
- 检验标本采集手册
- 陆上石油天然气长输管道建设项目安全设施设计编制导则(试行)2015
- 神经系统疾病概述课件
- 新高考背景下2025届高考地理一轮复习备考策略讲座
- 中医海外传播
- 文创产品国内外研究现状综述
评论
0/150
提交评论